Parents of young children may want to take note of our findings. Infants are not just learning to eat the foods they are given; they are also learning by watching adults eat, and figuring out who eats what foods with whom. By introducing babies to social contexts in which adults make healthful food choices, parents may help children learn the cultural norms of healthful eating themselves.
Babies Watching People Eat
Sunday
Review / New York Times | 19 August 2016
You may
not be surprised to learn that food preference is a social matter. What we
choose to eat depends on more than just what tastes good or is healthful.
People in different cultures eat different things, and within a culture, what
you eat can signal something about who you are.
More surprising is that the
sociality of food selection, it turns out, runs deep in human nature. In research
published this month in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, my
colleagues and I showed that even 1-year-old
babies understand that people’s food preferences depend on their social or
cultural group.
Interestingly, we
found that babies’ thinking about food preferences isn’t really about food per
se. It’s more about the people eating foods, and the relationship between food choice and social
groups.
Using this method, the
psychologists Zoe Liberman, Amanda Woodward, Kathleen Sullivan and I conducted
a series of studies. Led by Professor Liberman, we brought more than 200
1-year-olds (and their parents) into a developmental psychology lab, and showed
them videos of people visibly expressing like or dislike of foods.
For instance, one group of
babies saw a video of a person who ate a food and expressed that she loved it.
Next they saw a video of a second person who tried the same food and also loved
it. This second event was not terribly surprising to the babies: The two people
agreed, after all. Accordingly, the babies did not look for very long at this
second video; it was what they expected.
But when the babies saw the
second person do something less expected — when this second person instead
hated this same food that the first person loved — the babies looked much
longer.
In this
way, we were able to gauge infants’ patterns of generalization from one person
to another. If babies see someone like a food, do they think that other people
will like that food, too? And if so, do they think that all people will like
the same foods, or just some people?
We found some surprising
patterns. If the two people featured acted as if they were friends, or if they
spoke the same language, babies expected that the people would prefer the same
foods. But if the two people acted as if they were enemies, or if they spoke
two different languages, babies expected that they would prefer two different
foods.
It was as if cultural lines
were being drawn right in the laboratory. And in the babies’ minds there seemed
to be something special about the link between culture and food: When the
babies saw people liking and disliking inedible objects, we didn’t observe the
same patterns of results.
One thing you may be wondering
— and we were, too — is whether this is all about the foods people like. Whether you like grits
or kale may depend on cultural identity. But there are some things that are
disgusting to all humans, regardless of culture. Do babies intuitively know
this?
Indeed, they seem to. When the
babies in our studies saw a person act disgusted from eating a food, they
expected that a second person would also be disgusted by the same food —
regardless of whether or not the two people were in the same social group.
We also discovered something
interesting about what babies identify as meaningful cultural differences.
Babies from monolingual English-speaking homes saw language as a marker of
different cultures; as noted above, if two people spoke two different
languages, babies expected that they would prefer two different foods.
In contrast, babies from
bilingual homes assumed that even two people who spoke different languages
would like to eat the same things. Thus babies have the potential to learn
different things about the foods and people around them, depending on their
social environments.
Parents of young
children may want to take note of our findings. Infants are not just learning
to eat the foods they are given; they are also learning by watching adults eat,
and figuring out who eats what foods with whom. By introducing babies to social
contexts in which adults make healthful food choices, parents may help children
learn the cultural norms of healthful eating themselves.
No comments:
Post a Comment