Peace is not the result of integrated coexistence. It is the result of well-defined geographic and political boundaries.
Social Science Palooza IV
A day without social science is like a day without sunshine. Fortunately, every morning Kevin Lewis of National Affairs magazine
gathers recent social science findings and emails them out to the
masses. You can go to the National Affairs website to see and sign up
for his work, but, in the meantime, here are some recent interesting
findings:
Working moms sometimes raise smarter students.
Caitlin McPherran Lombardi and Rebekah Levine Coley studied the
children of mothers who work and those of mothers who don’t. They found
the children of working mothers were just as ready for school as other
children. Furthermore, among families where the father’s income was
lower, the children of working mothers demonstrated higher cognitive
skills and fewer conduct problems than the children of nonworking
mothers. As with all this work, no one study is dispositive, but here is
some more support for the idea that mothers who work are not hurting
their kids.
The office is often a more relaxing place than the home. Sarah Damaske, Joshua Smyth and Matthew Zawadzki found that people are more likely to have lower values of the stress hormone cortisol when they are at work than when at home. Maybe that’s because parenting small kids is so demanding. But, on the contrary: Having children around was correlated with less relative stress at home.
Hearts and minds may be a myth.
Armies fighting counterinsurgency campaigns spend a lot of effort
trying to win over the hearts and minds of the local populations. But
Raphael Cohen looked at polling data from Vietnam, Iraq and Afghanistan
and found that public opinion is a poor predictor of strategic victory.
Public opinion is not that malleable, and its swings are more an effect
than a cause. That is, counterinsurgency armies get more popular as they
win victories; they don’t get popular and then use that popularity to
win.
Attractive children attract less empathy than unattractive children.
Robert Fisher and Yu Ma studied how much help children received from
unrelated adults when they were experiencing difficulties. People
perceive that attractive children are more socially competent and,
therefore, are less likely to help them, as long as the need is not
severe. So, if you are creating an ad to get people to donate to your
hospital or charity, you might avoid child models who are winners in the
looks department.
Too much talent can be as bad as too little talent.
Most people assume there is a linear relationship between talent and
team performance. But Roderick Swaab and others studied team performance
in basketball and found that more talent is better up to a point —
after which more talent just means worse teamwork and ultimately worse
performance. In baseball, more talent did lead to better team
performance straight up the line, but in activities like basketball,
which require more intra-team coordination, too much talent can tear
apart teamwork.
Title IX has produced some unintended consequences.
Phoebe Clarke and Ian Ayres studied the effect of sports on social
outcomes. They found that a 10 percentage point increase in state level
female sports participation generated a 5 or 6 percentage point rise in
the rate of female secularism, a 5 point rise in the proportion of women
who are mothers and a 6 point rise in the percentage who are single
mothers. It could be that sports participation is correlated with
greater independence from traditional institutions, with good and bad
effects.
Moral stories don’t necessarily make more moral children.
Kang Lee, Victoria Talwar and others studied the effectiveness of
classic moral stories in promoting honesty among 3- to 7-year-olds. They
found stories like “Pinocchio” and “The Boy Who Cried Wolf” failed to
reduce lying in children. However, the story of “George Washington and
the Cherry Tree” significantly increased truth-telling. Stories that
emphasized the bad effects of lying had no effect, but stories that
emphasized the good effects of telling the truth did have an effect.
Good fences make good neighbors.
When ethnic groups clash, we usually try to encourage peace by
integrating them. Let them get to know one another or perform a joint
activity. This may be the wrong approach. Alex Rutherford, Dion Harmon
and others studied ethnically diverse areas and came to a different
conclusion. Peace is not the result of integrated coexistence. It is the
result of well-defined geographic and political boundaries. For
example, Switzerland is an ethnically diverse place, but mountains and
lakes clearly define each group’s spot. Even in the former Yugoslavia,
amid widespread ethnic violence, peace prevailed where there were clear
boundaries.
Most
social science research confirms the blindingly obvious. But sometimes
it reveals things nobody had thought of, or suggests that the things we
thought were true are actually false.
That’s a message for you, federal appropriators.
No comments:
Post a Comment