The Archipelago of Pain
International New York Times | 6 March 2014
We
don’t flog people in our prison system, or put them in thumbscrews or
stretch them on the rack. We do, however, lock prisoners away in social
isolation for 23 hours a day, often for months, years or decades at a
time.
We
prohibit the former and permit the latter because we make a distinction
between physical and social pain. But, at the level of the brain where
pain really resides, this is a distinction without a difference. Matthew
Lieberman of the University of California, Los Angeles, compared the
brain activities of people suffering physical pain with people suffering
from social pain. As he writes in his book, “Social,” “Looking at the
screens side by side ... you wouldn’t have been able to tell the
difference.”
The
brain processes both kinds of pain in similar ways. Moreover, at the
level of human experience, social pain is, if anything, more traumatic,
more destabilizing and inflicts more cruel and long-lasting effects than
physical pain. What we’re doing to prisoners in extreme isolation, in
other words, is arguably more inhumane than flogging.
If
there is communication with the prison staff, it might take place
through an intercom. Communication with the world beyond is minimal. If
there are visitors, conversation may be conducted through a video
screen. Prisoners may go years without affectionately touching another
human being. Their only physical contact will be brushing up against a
guard as he puts on shackles for trips to the exercise yard.
In
general, mammals do not do well in isolation. In the 1950s, Harry
Harlow studied monkeys who had been isolated. The ones who were isolated
for longer periods went into emotional shock, rocking back and forth.
One in six refused to eat after being reintegrated and died within five
days. Most of the rest were permanently withdrawn.
Studies
on birds, rats and mice consistently show that isolated animals suffer
from impoverished neural growth compared with socially engaged animals,
especially in areas where short-term memory and threat perception are
processed. Studies on Yugoslav prisoners of war in 1992 found that those
who had suffered blunt blows to the head and those who had been
socially isolated suffered the greatest damage to brain functioning.
Some
prisoners who’ve been in solitary confinement are scarcely affected by
it. But this is not typical. The majority of prisoners in solitary
suffer severely — from headaches, an oversensitivity to stimuli,
digestion problems, loss of appetite, self-mutilation, chronic
dizziness, loss of the ability to concentrate, hallucinations, illusions
or paranoid ideas.
The
psychiatrist Stuart Grassian conducted in-depth interviews with more
than 200 prisoners in solitary and concluded that about a third
developed acute psychosis with hallucinations. Many people just
disintegrate. According to rough estimates, as many as half the suicides
in prison take place in solitary, even though isolated prisoners make
up only about 5 percent of the population.
Prison
officials argue that they need isolation to preserve order. That’s a
view to be taken seriously because these are the people who work in the
prisons. But the research on the effectiveness of solitary confinement
programs is ambiguous at best. There’s a fair bit of evidence to suggest
that prison violence is not produced mainly by a few bad individuals
who can be removed from the mainstream. Rather, violence is caused by
conditions and prison culture. If there’s crowding, tension, a culture
of violence, and anarchic or arbitrary power, then the context itself is
going to create violence no matter how many “bad seeds” are segregated
away.
Fortunately,
we seem to be at a moment when public opinion is turning. Last month,
the executive director of the Colorado prisons, Rick Raemisch, wrote a moving first-person Op-Ed article
in The Times about his short and voluntary stay in solitary. Colorado
will no longer send prisoners with severe mental illnesses into
solitary. New York officials recently agreed to new guidelines limiting
the time prisoners can spend in isolation. Before long, one suspects,
extreme isolation will be considered morally unacceptable.
The
larger point is we need to obliterate the assumption that inflicting
any amount of social pain is O.K. because it’s not real pain.
When
you put people in prison, you are imposing pain on them. But that
doesn’t mean you have to gouge out the nourishment that humans need for
health, which is social, emotional and relational.
No comments:
Post a Comment