The Mystery Why You Can't Remember Being a Baby
Babies are sponges for new information – so why does it take so long for us to form your first memory? BBC Future investigates.
In Depth | BBC Future | 26 July 2016
You’re out to
lunch with someone you’ve known for a few years. Together you’ve held parties,
celebrated birthdays, visited parks and bonded over your mutual love of ice
cream. You’ve even been on holiday together. In all, they’ve spent quite a lot
of money on you – roughly £63,224.
The thing is: you can’t remember any of it.
From the most
dramatic moment in life – the day of your birth – to first steps, first words,
first food, right up to nursery school, most of us can’t remember anything of
our first few years. Even after our precious first memory, the recollections
tend to be few and far between until well into our childhood. How come?
This gaping hole
in the record of our lives has been frustrating parents and baffling
psychologists, neuroscientists and linguists for decades. It was a minor obsession of the father of psychotherapy, Sigmund
Freud, who coined the phrase ‘infant amnesia’ over 100 years ago.
Probing that
mental blank throws up some intriguing questions. Did your earliest memories
actually happen, or are they simply made up? Can we remember events without the
words to describe them? And might it one day be possible to claim your missing
memories back?
Babies are sponges, absorbing information at an astonishing rate - yet they fail to form clear memories of events (Credit: simpleInsomnia/Flickr/CC-BY-2.0) |
Part of the puzzle
comes from the fact that babies are, in other ways, sponges for new
information, forming 700 new neural connections every second and wielding
language-learning skills to make the most accomplished polyglot green with
envy. The latest research suggests they begin training
their minds before they’ve even left the womb.
But even as
adults, information is lost over time if there’s no attempt to retain it. So
one explanation is that infant amnesia is simply a result of the natural
process of forgetting the things we experience throughout our lives.
An answer comes
from the work of the 19th Century German psychologist Hermann Ebbinghaus, who
conducted a series of pioneering experiments on himself to test the limits of
human memory. To ensure his mind was a completely blank slate to begin with, he
invented the “nonsense syllable” – a made-up word of random letters, such as
“kag” or “slans” – and set to work memorising thousands of them.
His forgetting curve charts the disconcertingly rapid
decline of our ability to recall the things we’ve learnt: left alone, our
brains throw away half of all new material within an hour. By Day 30, we’ve
retained about 2-3%.
Crucially,
Ebbinghaus discovered that the way we forget is entirely predictable. To find
out if babies’ memories are any different, all we have to do is compare the
charts. When they did the maths in the 1980s, scientists discovered we recall far fewer memories between
birth and the age of six or seven than you would expect. Clearly something very
different was going on.
Our culture can detemine how our memories form and develop (Credit: simpleInsomnia/Flickr/CC BY 2.0) |
Intriguingly, the
veil lifts earlier for some than for others. Some people can remember events
from when they were just two years old,
while others may have no recollection of anything that has happened to them for
seven or eight years.
On average, patchy footage appears from about three-and-a-half. More
intriguingly still, discrepancies in forgetting have also been observed from
country to country, where the average onset of our earliest memories can vary
by up to two years.
Could this offer
some clues to explain the blank beforehand? To find out, psychologist Qi Wang
at Cornell University collected hundreds
of memories from Chinese and American college students. As the national
stereotypes would predict, American stories were longer, more elaborate and
conspicuously egocentric. Chinese stories, on the other hand, were briefer and
more factual; on average, they also began six months later.
It’s a pattern
backed up by numerous other studies. Those with more detailed,
self-focused memories seem to find them easier to recall. It’s thought that a
dash of self-interest can be helpful, since developing your own perspective
infuses events with meaning. “It is the difference between thinking ‘There were
tigers at the zoo’ and ‘I saw tigers at the zoo and even though they were
scary, I had a lot of fun’,” says Robyn Fivush, a psychologist at Emory
University.
Wang’s first
memory is of hiking in the mountains around her family home in Chongqing,
China, with her mother and her sister. She was about six. The thing is, until
she moved to the US, she’d never been asked. “In Eastern cultures childhood
memories aren’t important. People are like ‘why do you care?’” she says.
Some psychologists argue that the ability to form vivid autobiographical memories only comes with the power of speech (Credit: Kimberly Hopkins/Flickr/CC By 2.0) |
“If society is
telling you those memories are important to you, you’ll hold on to them,” says
Wang. The record for the earliest memories goes to Maori New Zealanders, whose
culture includes a strong emphasis on the past. Many can recall events which
happened when they were just two-and-a-half.
Our culture may
also determine the way we talk about our memories, with some psychologists
arguing that they only come once we have mastered the power of speech.
“Language helps provide a structure, or organisation, for our memories, that is
a narrative. By creating a story, the experience becomes more organised,
and therefore easier to remember over time,” says Fivush. Some psychologists
are sceptical that this plays much of a role, however. There’s no difference
between the age at which children who are born deaf and grow up without sign
language report their earliest memories, for instance.
After a botched operation to cure his epilepsy damaged his hippocampus, HM was unable to recall any new events
This leads us to
the theory that we can’t remember our first years simply because our brains
hadn’t developed the necessary equipment. The explanation emerges from the most
famous man in the history of neuroscience, known simply as patient HM. After a
botched operation to cure his epilepsy damaged his hippocampus, HM was unable
to recall any new events. “It’s the centre of our ability to learn and
remember. If it weren’t for the hippocampus I wouldn’t be able to remember this
conversation now,” says Jeffrey Fagen, who studies memory and learning at St
John's University.
Intriguingly,
however, he was still able to learn other kinds of information – just like
babies. When scientists asked him to copy a drawing of a five-pointed star by
looking at it in a mirror (harder than it sounds), he improved with each round
of practise – despite the fact the experience itself felt completely new to
him.
We can't always trust our early memories to be accurate - sometimes they will have been moulded by later conversations about the event (Credit: simpleInsomnia/Flickr/CC By 2.0) |
Perhaps, when
we’re very young, the hippocampus simply isn’t developed enough to build a rich
memory of an event. Baby rats, monkeys and humans all continue to add new neurons to the hippocampus
for the first few years of life and we all are all unable to form lasting
memories as infants – and it seems that the moment we stop creating new
neurons, we‘re suddenly able to form long-term memories. “For young babies and
infants the hippocampus is very undeveloped,” says Fagen.
But is the
under-formed hippocampus losing our long-term memories, or are they never
formed in the first place? Since childhood events can continue to affect our behaviour long after we’ve
forgotten them, some psychologists think they must be lingering somewhere. “The
memories are probably stored someplace that’s inaccessible now, but it’s very
difficult to demonstrate that empirically,” says Fagen.
We should be very
wary about what we do recall from that time, though – our childhood is probably
full of false memories for events that never occurred.
Elizabeth Loftus,
a psychologist at the University of California, Irvine, has devoted her career
to the phenomenon. “People can pick up suggestions and begin to visualise them
– they become like memories,” she says.
Imaginary events
Loftus knows
first-hand how easily this happens. Her mother drowned in a swimming pool when
she was just 16. Years later, a relative convinced her that she had discovered
her floating body. It all came flooding back, until a week later the same
relative called and explained she’d got it wrong – it was someone else.
Of course, no one
likes to be told their memories aren’t real. To convince the sceptics, Loftus
knew she’d need unequivocal proof. Back in the 1980s, she recruited volunteers
for a study and planted the memories herself.
Loftus spun an elaborate lie about a traumatic
trip to a shopping mall when they got lost, before being rescued by a kindly
elderly woman and reunited. To make the event more plausible, she even roped in
their families. “We basically said to our research participants ‘we’ve talked
to your mother, your mother has told us some things that happened to you.’”
Nearly a third of her victims fell for it, with some apparently recalling the
event in vivid detail. In fact, we’re often more confident in our imaginary memories than we are
in those which actually happened.
Even if your
memories are based on real events, they have probably been moulded and
refashioned in hindsight – memories planted by conversations rather than
first-person memories of the actual events. That time you thought it would be
funny to turn your sister into a zebra with
permanent marker? You saw it in a family video. The incredible third birthday
cake your mother made you? Your older brother told you about it.
Perhaps the
biggest mystery is not why we can’t remember our childhood – but whether we can
believe any of our memories at all.
No comments:
Post a Comment