A woman pauses along the edge of the North Pool of the 9/11 Memorial in New York |
Memory Lane Has a Three-Way Fork
The Atlantic | 25 October 2016
Three different parts of the brain control how successfully, accurately, and vividly we remember past events.
In his magnum opus, In Search of Lost Time, Marcel
Proust wrote that “remembrance of things past is not necessarily the
remembrance of things as they were.” That elegant line speaks to a simple
truth: There are things you remember, and there are things you remember well. Even if you can recall
a past event, your memories will vary considerably in how much detail they
contain, and how correct those details are.
In an elegant experiment, a
team of neuroscientists led by Jon Simons at the
University of Cambridge have shown that these aspects of our memories—our
success at recalling them, their precision, and their vividness—depend on three
different parts of the brain.
One of these is the
hippocampus—the little, seahorse-shaped area in the middle of the brain that
has been most famously associated with memory. In 1953, a neurosurgeon named
William Beecher Scoville removed the hippocampus from an epileptic patient
named Henry Molaison, robbing him of many past memories of events and
preventing him from making any new ones. Molaison became known as Patient HM, and his
mental woes enshrined the hippocampus as “the seat of memory.”
It doesn’t act alone. Through brain-scanning studies, scientists
have uncovered a network of regions involved in remembering what happened to
us—the hippocampus, yes, but also regions further back in the brain, like the
angular gyrus and precuneus. When volunteers try to bring up old memories,
these areas all start buzzing together.
But that doesn't necessarily
mean these regions are all doing the same thing. If people suffer damage to the
hippocampus, as Henry Molaison did, they typically can’t remember anything at
all. But in 2010, Simons found that people who suffer damage further back in
the brain experience subtler problems—they’ll remember things
well enough, but not confidently so. A few years later, he used magnetic
fields to temporarily disrupt these areas in healthy volunteers and found the
same effect: The recruits remembered
accurately, but not confidently. That was a strong hint that different
parts of the memory experience are governed by different parts of the brain.
To explore this idea,
Simons’s team members Franziska Richter and Rose Cooper devised a
new test. They asked volunteers to memorize pictures in which distinctive
objects—say, a purple umbrella or an orange flashlight—floated in front of
various background scenes, from the Taj Mahal to a lava field. The images
disappeared and after a short pause, the recruits had to say how vividly they
remembered the scene. Then, they had to recreate the images, adding whichever
objects they remembered back to an empty background, and trying to match their
color, orientation, and location. And they did all of this inside an fMRI
scanner, which gave Richter and Cooper a view inside their heads.
Richter
and Cooper found that anytime the volunteers remembered
objects, their hippocampus was more active. When their memories were precise,
and they nailed the objects’ features, their angular gyrus was abuzz with
activity. And when they claimed that their memories were especially vivid, it
was their precuneus that lit up in the scanners. Three areas, three different
aspects of memory.
This might seem like a deeply
counter-intuitive idea. We’re very used to thinking of our memory as a kind of
storage vault, where bits of information are recorded and filed away for later
perusal. But it’s not like that at all.
Instead, many neuroscientists
believe that “our memories are reconstructed whenever we try to remember
something,” says Cooper. We build them from scratch each and every time,
drawing on bits of information scattered in relevant corners of the brain.
Sights come in from the visual centers, sounds from the auditory centers, and
all of it gets re-integrated in areas like the hippocampus. Memory isn’t just
an act of retrieval, but of reconstruction.
What
about the precuneus? “We found that the most difficult region to get our head
around,” says Cooper. It seems to govern the subjective side of memory, and
“the degree to which participants perceived that they could remember an event,
regardless of whether they could or not.” So, very roughly speaking, the
hippocampus kicks the process of, the angular gyrus does the heavy lifting, and
the precuneus gives us that vibrant, first-person sense of actually remembering
something.
And of course, these areas are
heavily interconnected, so that activity in one affects the function of the
others. By their powers combined, we remember.
“I like this study a lot,” says
Elizabeth Chua from
Brooklyn College of the City University of New York. “One challenge with
studying memory success is that there are many factors that can contribute to
success.” By teasing those factors apart in a single study, the team could more
effectively work out what different brain regions that have been linked to
memory actually do.
It’s further evidence that
“memory retrieval is not all or nothing,” adds Maureen Ritchey from
Boston College. “We can remember some details of an experience but not others,
and the details we do recall can be quite specific or disappointingly vague.”
And by studying how these subtleties are encoded in the brains of healthy
volunteers, Simons’ team might be well-placed to understand what happens in
people whose memories are failing.
There are several neurological
and developmental disorders where people don’t lose their memories outright,
but show subtler problems. “It's possible that the kind of task [the team used]
could be more sensitive to changes in memory than other standard memory tasks,”
adds Ritchey. And perhaps doctors could use those changes to spot problems at
an early stage.
No comments:
Post a Comment